Friday, August 23, 2013

Memory

Knowledge Issues  
To what extent is memory reliable?
To what extent do our senses give an accurate picture of the world?

Analytical Approaches
Examining the validity of drawing generalized conclusions from specific experiments
Examining the impact of language – questions in particular – on recall.
Determining cause and effect
Examining methodological issues
What are the ramifications of this research for establishing the validity of eye witness testimony in court?

How easily can memory be altered?
  



Study 1: 'Reconstruction of automobile destruction' Loftus and Palmer (1974)

BACKGROUND

There is much support for the idea that most people, when they are witnesses to a complex event, such as a traffic accident, are very inaccurate when reporting numerical details like time, distance, and especially speed, even when they know that they will be questioned on them (e.g. Marshall, 1969). As a consequence, there can sometimes be large variations in estimates between witnesses and so it seems likely that such inaccurate testimony could easily be influenced by variables such as the phrasing of questions or 'leading' questions.

AIM

Loftus and Palmer, therefore, aimed to investigate the effect of leading questions on the accuracy of speed estimates in, and perceived consequences of, a car crash.

EXPERIMENT ONE

Subjects: 45 students, tested in groups of different sizes.   
Design: Laboratory experiment.

Procedure:

7 films of traffic accidents, ranging in duration from 5 to 30 seconds, were presented in a random order to each group.

After each film, the subjects had to give a general account of what they had just seen and then answer more specific questions about the accident. The critical question, 'About how fast were the cars going when they hit each other?' acted as the independent variable, since it was manipulated in five conditions.. Nine subjects heard the sentence with the verb 'hit' in it, and then an equal number of the remaining subjects were asked the same question but with the verb 'smashed', 'collided', 'bumped' or 'contacted' instead of 'hit'. The estimated speed was the dependent variable.

Results
Speed estimates for the verbs of experiment one

Verb    Mean speed estimate

Smashed    40.8
Collided    39.3
Bumped    38.1
Hit    34.0
Contacted    31.8

Significance of result

Results were significant at the P < .005 level, according to analysis of variance of the data.

Accuracy of subjects' speed estimates

In 4 of the 7 films the speed of the cars was known.

Actual speed of collision     Mean speed estimate
Film 1    20 mph                  37.7 mph
Film 2    30 mph                  36.2 mph
Film 3    40 mph                 39.7 mph
Film 4    40 mph                 36.1 mph


Discussion

The results indicate that not only are people poor judges of speed, but they are systematically and significantly affected by the wording of a question.

However, it is unclear if this finding could be attributed to either response-bias (the subject remembers accurately but is pressured by the word to increase or decrease the estimate) or a genuine change in the subject's memory of the event (the word makes the subject recall the event as worse than it was).

Therefore, this study suffers from a lack of validity. There is a lack of evidence that suggests memory is being altered as the subjects could simply be changing their account to suit the needs of the researcher but the memory remains accurately intact.

Therefore, further testing was necessary into whether the subject might be led into recalling details that did not occur. The second experiment was designed to specifically test memory accuracy.



EXPERIMENT TWO

Subjects: 150 students, tested in groups of different sizes.
Design: Laboratory experiment.

Procedure:

A film lasting just less than a minute was presented to each group which featured four seconds of a multiple traffic accident. After the film, the subjects had to give a general account of what they had just seen and then answer more specific questions about the accident. The critical question concerning the speed of the cars was the independent variable, and it was manipulated by asking 50 subjects 'About how fast were the cars going when they hit each other?', another 50 'About how fast were the cars going when they smashed into each other?' and another 50 acted as a control group who were not asked the question at all. One week later the dependent variable was measured - without seeing the film again they answered ten questions, one of which was a critical one randomly positioned in amongst the ten questions, asking 'Did you see any broken glass? Yes or no?'. Although there was no broken glass it was expected that some might be falsely remembered if the leading question of a week ago had changed the memory of the event to seem worse than it was.

Results

Verb    Mean estimate   
Smashed    10.46 mph
Hit              8.00 mph

Response     Smashed     Hit     Control
Saw broken glass    16       7        6
Did not see glass    34       43       44

Probability of seeing broken glass with speed estimate
Verb    1-5 mph     6-10 mph     11-15 mph    16-20 mph
Smashed    .09       .27             .  41                  .62
Hit              .06       .09              .25                   .50



Discussion

The authors conclude that the results show that the verb 'smashed' not only increases the estimates of speed, but also the likelihood of seeing broken glass that was not present. This indicates that information from the original memory is merged with information after the fact, producing one distorted memory. This shift in memory representations in line with verbal cues has received support from other research.

EVALUATION

Methodological: A well operationalised and controlled experiment, but lacked the ecological validity of having real life events and involved witnesses.

However, there is a key problem with internal validity (the results may have an alternative explanation than what the authors claim). For example: the results show the higher the speed, the more likely the participants claim they saw broken glass. This may be due to a desire to appear consistent with their earlier responses: If they had estimated a higher speed then it is logical for them to assume there will have been broken glass. They 'took a chance' that broken glass was present which was a logical assumption after estimating a higher speed but the study does not necessarily show a failure in memory.

In the introduction of their study the authors claim to be measuring memory by asking:

"How accurately do we remember the details of a complex event, like a traffic accident, that has happened in our presence?" (1974: 585, italics mine)

However, in their discussion they acknowledge memory may not be at fault but the answer given:
 
"To reiterate, we have first of all provided an additional demonstration of something that has been known for some time, namely, that the way a question is asked can enormously influence the answer that is given." (1974: 588, italics mine)


Therefore they implicitly acknowledge the difference between answers and memory but they do not distinguish between them in the study.

However, it is not discussed further.

Therefore, the author's claim of reconstructive memory can be challenged by suggesting that rather than memory being altered it is the answers that are changing. This is a moot point in courts of law but it is a key point of validity for social scientists: What is being manipulated, the actual memory, recall or responses of participants under experimental conditions?  



Study 2: The effects of coherence and culture on reconstructing memory
War of the Ghosts
Aim: To investigate the effect of schemas on memory (Bartlett, 1932)
Method: Bartlett asked English participants to read The War of the Ghosts, a Native American folk tale containing details about hunting seals in canoes.
The participants’ memory for this story was tested by, serial reproduction and repeated reproduction. In serial reproduction, the first participant reads the original story and then reproduces it on paper. The first participant’s reproduction is read by the second participant who also reproduces it for a third participant. This procedure continues until six or seven reproductions are completed by an equal number of participants. In repeated reproduction, the same participant writes all six or seven reproductions over many years.
Results: The two methods led to very similar findings; the story became increasingly shorter and changed to suit the English cultural background of the participants. For example, ‘hunting seals’ became ‘fishing’ and ‘canoes’ became ‘boats’. What started as a very strange story to the participants became a traditional English story.


Study 3: Age effects on eye witness testimony.
Parker and Carranza (2005) 

Research focusing on the accuracy of elderly eyewitnesses has addressed two separate components of eyewitness memory: the ability of eyewitnesses to describe details of the event, and their ability to recognize the crime’s perpetrator

Aim: To investigate the effects of age of witness and age of suspect on eyewitness testimony

Method: Forty-eight elementary school children and 48 college students viewed a slide sequence of a mock crime. This was followed by target-present or target-absent photo identification.

Findings: In photo identification, child witnesses had a higher rate of choosing the correct crook than adult witnesses, suggesting that children have more lax criteria of responding and adults made significantly more 'don't know' choices. This supports the notion of schema development with age: Older people have more fixed expectations over what a crook should look like and so tried to fit what they remembered with their existing schema.


Concluding comments:
Memory is not a straightforward recall of the world as 'is' It is influenced by leading verbs, schemas and context. However, in all of these studies the 'gist' of the situation (e.g. car crash, office objects, burglary) was recalled correctly. Memory was influenced when researchers deliberately set out to influence it. Generalizability to everyday life is difficult. But some broad conclusions can be drawn: Memory is an active reconstructive process (as shown by War of the Ghosts), rather than a passive reproductive one